Where The Holistic Rubber Meets The Scientific Road

Incredible Facts about Memory You Need to Know

Do you know the basic (and fascinating) facts about memory?  Have you ever stopped to wonder what memory truly is and how it works?  It’s something we all take for granted.

In Search of an Engram

One of the first memory researchers was German biologist Richard Semon (1859- 1918). He wrote about active memory, used as a warning mechanism. An animal, say a dog, could experience an episode in which a boy bent down to pick up a stone, threw it at the dog, and there was pain from the impact.

Semon called this an engram, meaning a cellular trace memory. He also wrote explaining how these engrams later get re-stimulated or brought back to life in what he referred to as ecphoric stimulus – meaning the way a memory can be triggered spontaneously, by something similar in the person’s surroundings.

In the case of the boy and dog it might mean that every time someone bent down (say to tie his shoelaces) the dog would yelp in fright and run away, because the memory trace of being hit painfully by the stone was re-stimulated. The episode was similar to the one in which the boy bent down, to pick up a stone – just not identical.

Semon was brilliant in that he was the first to argue that memories weren’t just a recording function but must also include some retrieval function, otherwise we couldn’t get them back.

However, the retrieval aspect could malfunction and connect episodes that were really not the same; like people bending down are not all picking up stones to throw. Alfred Korzybski (1936) called this “identification thinking” or A=A=A.

Decades later, Karl Lashley published a famous paper entitled “In search of the engram.” He didn’t find it. Lashley sliced up living rat brains and no matter how much tissue he cut away, he couldn’t cut out any memory. With almost all brain tissue gone, the rats still remembered exactly what to do.

Where Does Memory Live…If Not the Brain?

This accords well with current human cases where individuals who have lost 98% or more of their brain tissue appear and behave normally, with memory intact. The condition which leads to this remarkable state of affairs is called hydrocephalus.

The fact is, the brain isn’t at all what lazy “scientists” claim it to be. At best, it simply functions as a communications interchange, between the private inner world of reality and the outer physical world. It processes memories to some degree but does not store them.

If memory is not in the brain, where is it?

The answer lies in the concept of an “extended mind.” You were taught the mind is in the body…specifically, in the brain. We are supposed to exist only in this 3-pound lump of jelly inside our skulls (it’s been christened the “Three-Pound Universe”).

A more recent model, pioneered by me for my Supernoetics incidentally, but written up and tested by Rupert Sheldrake, is that of the extended mind. It spreads outwards, beyond the skull.

Take out your cell phone and take a look at it (if you don’t have one, look at somebody else’s; they are everywhere in our environment). From that small device – much smaller than the brain – you can speak with a geographical spot almost anywhere on Earth. But that doesn’t mean the whole planet is inside the cell phone does it?

Yet that’s the kind of fact about memory that science tries to foist on us.

If you damage the brain or put it to sleep, the person is “gone.” That proves that we are just in the brain, they say. Yet if you turn off the cell phone, nobody seriously suggests that all places on the planet have vanished, do they?

Breakdown of Memory Facts

OK, let’s look at memory from the functional or biological viewpoint. There are other types of memory recognized, other than just the well-known long and short-term memory classification.

Declarative memory consists of facts and events that can be consciously recalled or “declared.” In other words, the memories are stored and retrieved. This is what most people mean when they talk of memory. Declarative memory is divided into episodic memory and semantic memory, and researcher Endel Tulving first proposed the distinction between the two in 1972.

One component of episodic memory is based on specific events, or “episodes” that are part of your personal history. Some examples are…

  • The town in which you were born
  • Your sister’s wedding
  • The name of your fifth-grade teacher
  • Where you were when John F. Kennedy was assassinated

The other component of declarative memory is semantic memory, which is the ability to recall facts and concepts, or what we would call “learning” in the education sense. Some examples are…

  • Understanding the difference between a dog and a cat
  • Knowing that the space shuttle Challenger disaster occurred on Jan. 28, 1986
  • Being able to associate letters with their sounds
  • Remembering history and dates

Procedural memory encompasses skills, which are learned and can be done automatically from memory.  Examples would be the use of tools or objects (like a musical instrument) or movements of the body that are deeply embedded and are performed without being aware (such as riding a bicycle).

Procedural memory is typically acquired through repetition and practice, sometimes described as muscle memory or body memory. Some examples of procedural memory are…

  • Playing a musical instrument
  • Ice skating
  • Driving a car
  • Shooting an arrow

These are tasks that you can go months or even years without performing and pick them up again quickly. Usually, amnesia impacts declarative memory only and has no effect on procedural memory. An amnesiac can remember how to talk drive a car but can’t recall with whom they spoke earlier that day.

How Declarative Memory Is Impacted

A 1997 study demonstrated that stress can have a significant impact on the formation of declarative memories. Declarative memory performance was worse in the participants that were exposed to a stressful situation after learning the words.

The chronic stress seen in those with post-traumatic stress disorder contributes to their inability to form declarative memories.

In 1953, a patient who had parts of his medial temporal lobe, hippocampus, and amygdala removed to address his epilepsy was studied to determine the impact it had on his procedural and declarative memory capabilities.

After the surgery, he could no longer form declarative memories but he was still able to form new procedural memories and short-term memories. This led to the understanding of the differences between procedural and declarative memories.

There have been a number of studies showing the impact of slow-wave sleep on our ability to form declarative memories. Declarative memory benefits mainly from sleep periods dominated by deep slow-wave sleep, while REM sleep doesn’t appear to enhance declarative memory.

To learn far more facts about memory, how your mind works, and some great ways to gain better control of your outcomes, it would be a good idea to watch my free video training series at New Thought Horizons

HOW TO CHANGE YOUR LIFE WITH CUTTING-EDGE MEMORY TECHNIQUES

New Thought Horizons is my exclusive transformative online training that can empower your mind beyond the boundaries of knowledge. I encourage you to join now and start on your journey of beneficial change and transformation.

MOST POPULAR ARTICLES

Most Trending Articles

Related Articles